Alzheimer’s disease is the leading cause of dementia in older persons yet few risk factors have been identified. The study being discussed, performed by Dr. Robert S. Wilson and colleagues, intends to test the hypothesis that frequent participation in cognitive activities is associated with a reduced risk of AD. The forthcoming information is extracted from
Cognitive activity is very hard to define considering people come from different cultural and socioeconomic backgrounds. Respondents therefore rated their current and past frequencies in activities that involved seeking or processing information. The participants were asked how much time each spent doing certain activities including: watching television, listening to radio, reading newspapers, reading magazines, reading books, playing games such as cards, checkers, crosswords, or other puzzles, and going to museums. Frequency was rated on a 5 point scale, 5 being everyday, and 1 being one time or less a year.
A scale from 1.57 to 4.71 (mean, 3.57) was observed as a measurement of cognitive activity with higher scores indicating more frequent activity. The results showed that during an average of 4.5 years follow-up, 111 participants developed Alzheimer’s disease. From the 1.57 – 4.71 cognitive activity scale, a 1 point increase was associated with a 33% reduction in risk of AD. Of the 111, 101 met NINCDS/ADRDA criteria for probable AD and 10 for possible AD (because of cognitive impairment due to stroke in 5 and to Parkinson disease in 5). Table 1 demonstrates baseline characteristics of those participants who did and did not develop AD.
The RR is set to change from a baseline of 1, for example, as the asterisk below the table points out, a 1 unit increase in cognitive activity score will show a .33 decrease in RR (i.e. decrease from baseline 1 to 0.67 = .33) and for every 1 year increase in age the RR increases by .14. So as an example for the participants, compared with a person with activity frequency at the 10th percentile (score = 2.86), the RR of AD was reduced by 28% in a person whose activity was in the 50th percentile (score = 3.71) and by 47% in a person whose activity was in the 90thpercentile (score = 4.29). Education was not related to disease risk in this model (i.e. change from baseline 1 to 1.02 was negligible). The article makes other comparisons and accounts for variables that needed to be addressed such as early onset AD, depression, physical activity, and the presence of the ApoE epsilon4 allele which is a known risk factor for AD.
Along with the study, cognitive activity was compared to the change in cognitive function starting at the manifestation of AD in the patients that had developed it. The analysis used the global measure of cognition, which ranged from -1.765 to 1.374 at baseline, with higher scores indicating better function. Each point of cognitive activity score was associated with 0.128 units in the global cognitive score. On average, the global cognitive score declined 0.043 units per year of evaluation and this rate decreased by 0.020 units (~47%) for each 1-point increase in cognitive activity score. Table 3 demonstrates the results.
So basically on average, a person with activity frequency at the 10th percentile declined 0.046 units per year in global cognition; the rate was reduced by 0.014 units (~30%) for an activity frequency score at the 50th percentile and by 0.026 units (~60%) for an activity frequency score at the 90th percentile. The table also shows correlations between cognitive activity and other domains of cognition. So on average performance declined in each domain, as shown by the terms for time. Every 1-unit increase in cognitive activity however showed a decrease in the decline rate as illustrated by the “cognitive activity x time” portions of the table. As is exemplified cognitive activity was associated with lower rates of decline in working memory and perceptual speed. The change in semantic memory and visuospatial ability was not significantly related to cognitive activity.
The study showed a correlation between cognitive activity and development of AD yet the association between the two is still uncertain. As directly stated in the article, one hypothesis is that cognitive activity is protective. One version of this hypothesis is that with repetition some cognitive skills become more efficient and less vulnerable to disruption by AD pathology. Alternatively, frequent cognitive activity may strengthen processing skills such as working memory and perceptual speed, which may help to compensate for age-related decline in other cognitive systems. So if cognitive activity is protective, reduced cognitive activity should be an early sign of disease.
Reference:
Wilson, Robert S., Carlos F. Mendes De Leon, Lisa L. Barnes, Julie A. Schneider, Julia A. Bienias, Denis A. Evans, and David A. Bennett. "Participation in Cognitively Stimulating Activities and Risk of Incident Alzheimer Disease." The Journal of the American Medical Association 287 (2002): 742-48. JAMA. Web.
I found the paper you posted to be very interesting. Wow, I should really get a boost on doing crosswords. But on the other hand I do have a tendency of reading. The hypothesis of the study was most interesting to me. It appears that the results could have a variety of interpretations. This is disappointing because it doesn't provide a cure or treatment for individuals with AD. This is just a glimmer of hope. I do think that people overall should get involved in activities that stimulate thinking. Like you said reading books, doing crosswords, and visiting museums might not be a cure but everyone should do it sometimes.
ReplyDeleteI am so glad that someone was able to find a study like this that we were discussing in class! This definitely makes sense to me. The more we exercise the brain, the more connections we make, and the more "bypasses" are available when some parts of the brain are not working. I think this study was HUGE with 801 participants and can have great applications for the clinical setting. Very interesting! Thanks for sharing!
ReplyDelete